As everybody knows, if you do not work out, your muscles get flaccid. What most people don’t realize, however, is that your brain also stays in better shape when you exercise. And not just challenging your noggin by, for example, learning a new language, doing difficult crosswords or taking on other intellectually stimulating tasks. As researchers are finding, physical exercise is critical to vigorous mental health, too. Surprised? Although the idea of exercising cognitive machinery by performing mentally demanding activities—popularly termed the “use it or lose it” hypothesis—is better known, a review of dozens of studies shows that maintaining a mental edge requires more than that. Other things you do—including participating in activities that make you think, getting regular exercise, staying socially engaged and even having a positive attitude—have a meaningful influence on how effective your cognitive functioning will be in old age. Further, the older brain is more plastic than is commonly known. At one time, the accepted stereotype was that “old dogs can’t learn new tricks.” Science has proved that this dictum must be discarded. Although older adults generally learn new pursuits more slowly than younger people do and cannot reach the peaks of expertise in a given field that they might have achieved if they had started in their youth, they nonetheless can improve their cognitive performance through effort—forestalling some of the declines in cognition that come with advancing age. As John Adams, one of the founding fathers and the second U.S. president, put it: “Old minds are like old horses; you must exercise them if you wish to keep them in working order.” The news comes at a propitious time. The proportion of older adults in the U.S. and in other industrial nations continues to grow: in 1900, 4.1 percent of U.S. citizens were older than 65; by 2000, that amount had jumped to 12.6 percent. By 2030, 20 percent of us will be in that category. From a societal point of view, prolonging independent functioning is both a desirable goal in itself and a way of deferring costs of long-term care. For individuals, maintaining optimal cognitive functioning is worthwhile simply because it promises to enhance our quality of life through the years.
Keeping muscles in shape also benefits cognition. Credit: Getty Images
Mental training How to keep minds keen over an entire life span is a question philosophers have mulled since the earliest writings on record. As Roman orator Cicero put it: “It is exercise alone that supports the spirits, and keeps the mind in vigor.” Modern research in this field began in the 1970s and 1980s, with studies that demonstrated that healthy older adults can improve performance to a greater extent than had been previously assumed. The earlier research did not fully address certain questions, such as how long adults could retain the new skills they had acquired through training, whether those specifically developed skills would also positively influence other areas of cognition needed in everyday life, and whether the studies done with small numbers of subjects would be broadly applicable. Cognitive training can lead to substantial benefits for older adults on the tasks trained, and some of these benefits are maintained over time and may transfer to other tasks. Toward the end of the 20th century the National Institute on Aging funded a consortium of researchers to conduct a large-scale training study in a sample of older Americans. In 2002 psychologist Karlene Ball of the University of Alabama at Birmingham and her colleagues published initial results on more than 2,500 individuals older than 65 who had received about 10 sessions of cognitive training. Participants were randomly assigned either to a cognitive-process training group to learn how to excel in one of three areas—memory, reasoning or visual search—or to a control group of subjects who did not receive training. At a follow-up two years later, the team randomly selected a set of the initial participants for booster training prior to evaluation. The results showed strong training-effect sizes in each group as compared with controls. In retests five years later, measurable training benefits were still present. More impressive, however, are recent training studies that focus on what psychologists call working memory and executive functions—how a person plans a strategic approach to a task, controls what is attended to, and how he or she manages the mind in the process. Psychologist Chandramallika Basak, then at the University of Illinois, and her colleagues showed that training in a real-time strategy video game that demands planning and executive control not only improved gaming performance but also enhanced performance on other tasks measuring aspects of executive control. Some studies have also increased the amount of practice provided. For instance, Florian Schmiedek and one of us (Lindenberger) of the Max Planck Institute for Human Development in Berlin and Martin Lövdén of the Karolinska Institute in Stockholm asked 101 younger and 103 older adults to practice 12 different tasks for 100 days. Each of the tasks belonged to one of three different sets of cognitive skills—working memory, episodic memory and perceptual speed. Both younger and older adults showed general improvements in working memory. Younger adults also broadly improved in memory and reasoning and maintained their gains in reasoning over a period of two years. You do not have to have specialized training, however, to achieve cognitive gains or ward off cognitive decline. Everyday activities such as reading can help. We reviewed evidence on activity-related cognitive enrichment in more than a dozen studies. In 2003 one of us (Wilson) and his colleagues at Rush University Medical Center in Chicago recruited more than 4,000 elderly residents from a geographically defined community and rated their frequency of participation in seven cognitive activities (for instance, reading magazines). At three-year intervals for a mean of nearly six years, participants completed an in-home interview that included brief tests of cognitive function. More frequent cognitive activity at the outset was associated with a reduced rate of cognitive decline over time. But not all studies of staying cognitively active have found such benefits. Getting physical Over the past decades several studies have underscored the link between physical activity and cognition. For instance, in a study published in 2001 neuropsychiatrist Kristine Yaffe of the University of California, San Francisco, and her colleagues recruited 5,925 women older than 65 at four different medical centers across the U.S. The participants were all free of any physical disability that would limit their ability to walk or pursue other physical activities. The volunteers were also screened to ensure that they did not have a cognitive impairment. The researchers then assessed their physical activity by asking the women how many city blocks they walked and how many flights of stairs they climbed daily and gave them a questionnaire to fill out about their levels of participation in 33 different physical activities. After six to eight years, the researchers assessed the women’s level of cognitive function. The most active women had a 30 percent lower risk of cognitive decline. Interestingly, walking distance was related to cognition, but walking speed was not. It seems that even moderate levels of physical activity can serve to limit declines in cognition. Moderate movement is good, but toning your circulatory system with aerobic exercise may be the real key to brain fitness. In a 1995 study of 1,192 healthy 70- to 79-year-olds, cognitive neuroscientist Marilyn Albert of Johns Hopkins University and her colleagues measured cognition with a battery of tasks that took approximately 30 minutes to complete and included tests of language, verbal memory, nonverbal memory, conceptualization and visuospatial ability. They found that the best predictors of cognitive change over a two-year period included strenuous activity and peak pulmonary expiratory flow rate. In an investigation published in 2004 epidemiologist Jennifer Weuve, then at Harvard University, and her colleagues also examined the relation between physical activity and cognitive change over a two-year period in 16,466 nurses who were older than 70. Participants logged how much time they spent per week in a variety of physical activities (running, jogging, walking, hiking, racket sports, swimming, bicycling, aerobic dance) over the past year and provided self-reports of walking pace in minutes per mile. Weuve’s group observed a significant relation between energy expended in physical activities and cognition, across a large set of cognitive measures. The research that we have described thus far has examined mental performance over relatively short periods—just several years. A few studies have begun to look at what happens over longer timescales. In 2005 Suvi Rovio, now at the University of Turku in Finland, and her colleagues examined the relation between physical activity at middle age and risk of dementia an average of 21 years later, when the cohort was between 65 and 79 years of age. Subjects indicated how often they participated in leisure-time physical activities that lasted at least 20 to 30 minutes and caused breathlessness and sweating. Conducting such activity at midlife at least twice a week was associated with a reduced risk of dementia in later life. Indeed, participants in the more active group had 52 percent lower odds of having dementia than the more sedentary group did. In 2010 Kirk Erickson and his colleagues at the University of Pittsburgh extended previous research examining the relation between physical activity, cognition and dementia by assessing self-reported physical activity alongside measures of regional brain volume. They reported an association between walking and retention of brain volume, which in turn predicted a reduced risk of dementia in 299 older adults over a period of 13 years. Interestingly, it is not just aerobic forms of physical activity (such as walking, jogging, swimming and bicycling) that have been associated with improvements in cognition. Teresa Liu-Ambrose, a professor of physical therapy at the University of British Columbia, reported in 2010 that resistance exercise, over a one-year period, improved aspects of executive control in older women.
Physical activity bolsters memory and thwarts dementia. Credit: CORBIS
Mind-body connection It makes sense that training or participation in mentally stimulating activities would help cognition, but it is perhaps less immediately obvious why physical activity would have such an effect. Consider the increasingly well-documented link between physical activity and disease. A plethora of studies have examined the health benefits of exercise and a nonsedentary lifestyle for prevention of disease. For example, we now know that physical activity reduces the risk of cardiovascular-related death, type 2 diabetes, colon and breast cancer, and osteoporosis. On the other hand, cardiovascular disease, diabetes and cancer have been associated with compromised cognition. Therefore, you might expect that increased physical activity and exercise would maintain cognition by reducing risk of diseases associated with cognitive decline. In a study published in 2006 psychologist Stanley J. Colcombe, then at the University of Illinois, and his colleagues examined the influence of fitness training on potential changes in brain structure. The six-month trial included 59 healthy but sedentary community-dwelling volunteers, age 60 to 79. Brain scans after fitness training showed that even relatively short exercise interventions can begin to restore some of the losses in brain volume associated with normal aging. In 2011 Erickson and his colleagues reported on work done at the University of Illinois, which showed that one year of walking, an hour a day for three days a week, increased the volume of the hippocampus in older adults. The hippocampus supports important aspects of memory such as associating an individual’s face with her name and discussions that you might have had with her. Researchers have found that different brain regions that need to communicate to support memory, reasoning and problem-solving become more poorly connected as we age. A study by Michelle Voss of the University of Illinois and her colleagues found, however, that when older adults participated in a six-month walking program, connections increased in a number of brain networks, and this increased connectivity was associated with improvements in executive-control processes (such as scheduling, planning, dealing with ambiguity, and working memory). Other recent studies have replicated and extended these results. These studies and those presented earlier in the article suggest improvement in cognition, brain structure and function in response to improvement in fitness. Supporting these findings, a large body of nonhuman animal research has demonstrated a number of changes in brain structure and function after animals are exposed to enriched, or complex, environments. Enriched environments usually include running wheels, a multitude of toys and objects to climb that are changed frequently, and animal companions. Exposure to such environments yields several physiological benefits. First, it increases the formation of new dendrite branches and synapses—the areas of neural cells that receive and send communication signals. It also increases the number of glial cells, which support the health of neurons, and expands the brain’s oxygen-supplying capillary network. Enriched environments foster the development of new neurons and create a cascade of molecular and neurochemical changes, such as an increase in neurotrophins—molecules that protect and grow the brain. Finally, physical activity in animals results in improvements in learning and memory.
Working on puzzles can help maintain mental acuity. Credit: Sally Anscombe Getty Images
Doing puzzles and push-ups are helpful for some—but other factors also boost mental fitness. For one, getting involved in social groups both improves cognition in general and seems to help thwart the arrival of dementia. The traditional focus of this research has been on relatively objective measures of social isolation versus connectedness, including the extent to which a person participates in activities that prominently involve social interaction (such as doing volunteer work), the number of friends and relatives an individual contacts regularly (in other words, the size of a person’s social network), and marital status. Findings about the positive aspects of attitudes and beliefs on adult cognition are spottier. In large part, positive beliefs and attitudes may have important indirect effects on cognitive enrichment because of their influence on the kinds of behaviors (for instance, exercise and mentally stimulating activities) that are known to be associated with cognitive enrichment. More generally, individuals who are optimistic, agreeable, open to new experiences, conscientious, positively motivated and goal-directed are more likely to undergo successful aging, to take advantage of opportunities, to cope effectively with life circumstances, to effectively regulate emotional reactions to events, and to maintain a sense of well-being and life satisfaction in the face of challenge. And just as maintaining some activity patterns in old age may reduce risk of cognitive decline, the persistence of other patterns of behavior may increase the risk. Chronic psychological distress—resulting from depression, anxiety, and negative emotions such as anger and shame—is associated with a variety of negative outcomes in adulthood, including cognitive decline. The tendency to experience psychological distress is often called neuroticism. Studies have consistently found a higher level of neuroticism to be linked to an increased incidence of Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in old age. Enriching cognition Clearly, there is no magic pill that inoculates the individual against cognitive decline in old age. Thus, public policy should follow a health prevention model. Policy leaders might promote intellectual activities that are inherently meaningful for older adults, perhaps as embedded in larger social contexts (for example, the Elderhostel movement or adult continuing education). A critical issue for future research will be to understand how an engaged way of life can be promoted and implemented in midlife, during the working years. Given inevitable conflicts between work demands and time available for other roles (parenting, for one) and activities, it would be useful to know whether work-related activity programs (such as availability and use of physical exercise facilities at or near the workplace) could help foster an enriching lifestyle. At the same time, the public must be aware that there is still much that is not known about cognitive fitness in old age, as well as some controversy about the magnitude and durability of mental exercise outcomes. People are marketing computer games and other means of exercising the mind, often making strong claims about the effectiveness of expensive products that have not been backed by actual scientific studies. Consumers should look for evidence demonstrating the benefits of such products—particularly with regard to everyday activities such as driving, memory for daily activities and better financial decision-making. The next decades offer much promise for expanding our knowledge about aging and cognition. We may soon discover whether the limits on successful cognitive functioning in old age that were once seen as insurmountable can ultimately be viewed as pessimistic assumptions that focused on observable age-related decline rather than the potential for maximizing human performance through cognitive enrichment. Just as advances in medical science may lead to increased longevity through vehicles such as effective treatments for dementia-causing illnesses, advances in psychological science can make important contributions to improving the quality of life of long-living older adults. In part, these advances will empirically demonstrate that attitudes and behaviors can promote cognitive functioning in old age and, more generally, show how behavioral interventions can help us all age successfully.
The Power of Walking Older adults who participated in aerobic exercise (walking) outperformed those in programs for stretching and toning (controls) in cognitive task areas: executive (related to planning and multitasking), controlled (effortful processes in response to novel situations), spatial (dealing with spatial information in perception or memory) and speed.
Source: “Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study,” by Stanley J. Colcombe and Arthur F. Kramer, in Psychological Science, Vol. 14, No. 2; March 2003
Capabilities over Time An individual’s cognitive function can vary from maturity into old age. Although good habits can promote sound thinking within a given range, we cannot completely halt the effects of aging. Top: Everyone experiences cognitive decline over a lifetime, but exercising the mind and body can help individuals stay above the middle (white dots) of the cognitive range (shaded area) and remain functional even beyond age 90. Bottom: In one study of practice benefits, older adults (mean age 72, blue) were slower than younger adults (mean age 21, red) to search memory (comparison slope, vertical axis) to verify whether a word was one of a set they had committed to memory a short time earlier. But with enough practice, they could speedily identify target words without needing to search for them, producing zero slopes such as those produced by younger people.
Source for graph 1: “Enrichment Effects on Adult Cognitive Development: Can the Functional Capacity of Older Adults Be Preserved and Enhanced?” by C. Hertzog et al., in Psychological Science in the Public Interest, Vol. 9, No. 1; October 2008, © 2009 Association for Psychological Science; Source for graph 2: “Aging and Individual Differences in the Development of Skilled Memory Search Performance,” by C. Hertzog et al., in Psychology and Aging, Vol. 11, No. 3; 1996
Surprised? Although the idea of exercising cognitive machinery by performing mentally demanding activities—popularly termed the “use it or lose it” hypothesis—is better known, a review of dozens of studies shows that maintaining a mental edge requires more than that. Other things you do—including participating in activities that make you think, getting regular exercise, staying socially engaged and even having a positive attitude—have a meaningful influence on how effective your cognitive functioning will be in old age.
Further, the older brain is more plastic than is commonly known. At one time, the accepted stereotype was that “old dogs can’t learn new tricks.” Science has proved that this dictum must be discarded. Although older adults generally learn new pursuits more slowly than younger people do and cannot reach the peaks of expertise in a given field that they might have achieved if they had started in their youth, they nonetheless can improve their cognitive performance through effort—forestalling some of the declines in cognition that come with advancing age. As John Adams, one of the founding fathers and the second U.S. president, put it: “Old minds are like old horses; you must exercise them if you wish to keep them in working order.”
The news comes at a propitious time. The proportion of older adults in the U.S. and in other industrial nations continues to grow: in 1900, 4.1 percent of U.S. citizens were older than 65; by 2000, that amount had jumped to 12.6 percent. By 2030, 20 percent of us will be in that category. From a societal point of view, prolonging independent functioning is both a desirable goal in itself and a way of deferring costs of long-term care. For individuals, maintaining optimal cognitive functioning is worthwhile simply because it promises to enhance our quality of life through the years.
Keeping muscles in shape also benefits cognition. Credit: Getty Images
Mental training
How to keep minds keen over an entire life span is a question philosophers have mulled since the earliest writings on record. As Roman orator Cicero put it: “It is exercise alone that supports the spirits, and keeps the mind in vigor.” Modern research in this field began in the 1970s and 1980s, with studies that demonstrated that healthy older adults can improve performance to a greater extent than had been previously assumed. The earlier research did not fully address certain questions, such as how long adults could retain the new skills they had acquired through training, whether those specifically developed skills would also positively influence other areas of cognition needed in everyday life, and whether the studies done with small numbers of subjects would be broadly applicable.
Cognitive training can lead to substantial benefits for older adults on the tasks trained, and some of these benefits are maintained over time and may transfer to other tasks. Toward the end of the 20th century the National Institute on Aging funded a consortium of researchers to conduct a large-scale training study in a sample of older Americans. In 2002 psychologist Karlene Ball of the University of Alabama at Birmingham and her colleagues published initial results on more than 2,500 individuals older than 65 who had received about 10 sessions of cognitive training. Participants were randomly assigned either to a cognitive-process training group to learn how to excel in one of three areas—memory, reasoning or visual search—or to a control group of subjects who did not receive training. At a follow-up two years later, the team randomly selected a set of the initial participants for booster training prior to evaluation. The results showed strong training-effect sizes in each group as compared with controls. In retests five years later, measurable training benefits were still present.
More impressive, however, are recent training studies that focus on what psychologists call working memory and executive functions—how a person plans a strategic approach to a task, controls what is attended to, and how he or she manages the mind in the process. Psychologist Chandramallika Basak, then at the University of Illinois, and her colleagues showed that training in a real-time strategy video game that demands planning and executive control not only improved gaming performance but also enhanced performance on other tasks measuring aspects of executive control.
Some studies have also increased the amount of practice provided. For instance, Florian Schmiedek and one of us (Lindenberger) of the Max Planck Institute for Human Development in Berlin and Martin Lövdén of the Karolinska Institute in Stockholm asked 101 younger and 103 older adults to practice 12 different tasks for 100 days. Each of the tasks belonged to one of three different sets of cognitive skills—working memory, episodic memory and perceptual speed. Both younger and older adults showed general improvements in working memory. Younger adults also broadly improved in memory and reasoning and maintained their gains in reasoning over a period of two years.
You do not have to have specialized training, however, to achieve cognitive gains or ward off cognitive decline. Everyday activities such as reading can help. We reviewed evidence on activity-related cognitive enrichment in more than a dozen studies. In 2003 one of us (Wilson) and his colleagues at Rush University Medical Center in Chicago recruited more than 4,000 elderly residents from a geographically defined community and rated their frequency of participation in seven cognitive activities (for instance, reading magazines). At three-year intervals for a mean of nearly six years, participants completed an in-home interview that included brief tests of cognitive function. More frequent cognitive activity at the outset was associated with a reduced rate of cognitive decline over time. But not all studies of staying cognitively active have found such benefits.
Getting physical
Over the past decades several studies have underscored the link between physical activity and cognition. For instance, in a study published in 2001 neuropsychiatrist Kristine Yaffe of the University of California, San Francisco, and her colleagues recruited 5,925 women older than 65 at four different medical centers across the U.S. The participants were all free of any physical disability that would limit their ability to walk or pursue other physical activities. The volunteers were also screened to ensure that they did not have a cognitive impairment. The researchers then assessed their physical activity by asking the women how many city blocks they walked and how many flights of stairs they climbed daily and gave them a questionnaire to fill out about their levels of participation in 33 different physical activities.
After six to eight years, the researchers assessed the women’s level of cognitive function. The most active women had a 30 percent lower risk of cognitive decline. Interestingly, walking distance was related to cognition, but walking speed was not. It seems that even moderate levels of physical activity can serve to limit declines in cognition.
Moderate movement is good, but toning your circulatory system with aerobic exercise may be the real key to brain fitness. In a 1995 study of 1,192 healthy 70- to 79-year-olds, cognitive neuroscientist Marilyn Albert of Johns Hopkins University and her colleagues measured cognition with a battery of tasks that took approximately 30 minutes to complete and included tests of language, verbal memory, nonverbal memory, conceptualization and visuospatial ability. They found that the best predictors of cognitive change over a two-year period included strenuous activity and peak pulmonary expiratory flow rate.
In an investigation published in 2004 epidemiologist Jennifer Weuve, then at Harvard University, and her colleagues also examined the relation between physical activity and cognitive change over a two-year period in 16,466 nurses who were older than 70. Participants logged how much time they spent per week in a variety of physical activities (running, jogging, walking, hiking, racket sports, swimming, bicycling, aerobic dance) over the past year and provided self-reports of walking pace in minutes per mile. Weuve’s group observed a significant relation between energy expended in physical activities and cognition, across a large set of cognitive measures.
The research that we have described thus far has examined mental performance over relatively short periods—just several years. A few studies have begun to look at what happens over longer timescales. In 2005 Suvi Rovio, now at the University of Turku in Finland, and her colleagues examined the relation between physical activity at middle age and risk of dementia an average of 21 years later, when the cohort was between 65 and 79 years of age. Subjects indicated how often they participated in leisure-time physical activities that lasted at least 20 to 30 minutes and caused breathlessness and sweating. Conducting such activity at midlife at least twice a week was associated with a reduced risk of dementia in later life. Indeed, participants in the more active group had 52 percent lower odds of having dementia than the more sedentary group did.
In 2010 Kirk Erickson and his colleagues at the University of Pittsburgh extended previous research examining the relation between physical activity, cognition and dementia by assessing self-reported physical activity alongside measures of regional brain volume. They reported an association between walking and retention of brain volume, which in turn predicted a reduced risk of dementia in 299 older adults over a period of 13 years. Interestingly, it is not just aerobic forms of physical activity (such as walking, jogging, swimming and bicycling) that have been associated with improvements in cognition. Teresa Liu-Ambrose, a professor of physical therapy at the University of British Columbia, reported in 2010 that resistance exercise, over a one-year period, improved aspects of executive control in older women.
Physical activity bolsters memory and thwarts dementia. Credit: CORBIS
Mind-body connection
It makes sense that training or participation in mentally stimulating activities would help cognition, but it is perhaps less immediately obvious why physical activity would have such an effect. Consider the increasingly well-documented link between physical activity and disease. A plethora of studies have examined the health benefits of exercise and a nonsedentary lifestyle for prevention of disease. For example, we now know that physical activity reduces the risk of cardiovascular-related death, type 2 diabetes, colon and breast cancer, and osteoporosis. On the other hand, cardiovascular disease, diabetes and cancer have been associated with compromised cognition. Therefore, you might expect that increased physical activity and exercise would maintain cognition by reducing risk of diseases associated with cognitive decline.
In a study published in 2006 psychologist Stanley J. Colcombe, then at the University of Illinois, and his colleagues examined the influence of fitness training on potential changes in brain structure. The six-month trial included 59 healthy but sedentary community-dwelling volunteers, age 60 to 79. Brain scans after fitness training showed that even relatively short exercise interventions can begin to restore some of the losses in brain volume associated with normal aging. In 2011 Erickson and his colleagues reported on work done at the University of Illinois, which showed that one year of walking, an hour a day for three days a week, increased the volume of the hippocampus in older adults. The hippocampus supports important aspects of memory such as associating an individual’s face with her name and discussions that you might have had with her.
Researchers have found that different brain regions that need to communicate to support memory, reasoning and problem-solving become more poorly connected as we age. A study by Michelle Voss of the University of Illinois and her colleagues found, however, that when older adults participated in a six-month walking program, connections increased in a number of brain networks, and this increased connectivity was associated with improvements in executive-control processes (such as scheduling, planning, dealing with ambiguity, and working memory). Other recent studies have replicated and extended these results. These studies and those presented earlier in the article suggest improvement in cognition, brain structure and function in response to improvement in fitness.
Supporting these findings, a large body of nonhuman animal research has demonstrated a number of changes in brain structure and function after animals are exposed to enriched, or complex, environments. Enriched environments usually include running wheels, a multitude of toys and objects to climb that are changed frequently, and animal companions. Exposure to such environments yields several physiological benefits. First, it increases the formation of new dendrite branches and synapses—the areas of neural cells that receive and send communication signals. It also increases the number of glial cells, which support the health of neurons, and expands the brain’s oxygen-supplying capillary network. Enriched environments foster the development of new neurons and create a cascade of molecular and neurochemical changes, such as an increase in neurotrophins—molecules that protect and grow the brain. Finally, physical activity in animals results in improvements in learning and memory.
Working on puzzles can help maintain mental acuity. Credit: Sally Anscombe Getty Images
Doing puzzles and push-ups are helpful for some—but other factors also boost mental fitness. For one, getting involved in social groups both improves cognition in general and seems to help thwart the arrival of dementia. The traditional focus of this research has been on relatively objective measures of social isolation versus connectedness, including the extent to which a person participates in activities that prominently involve social interaction (such as doing volunteer work), the number of friends and relatives an individual contacts regularly (in other words, the size of a person’s social network), and marital status. Findings about the positive aspects of attitudes and beliefs on adult cognition are spottier. In large part, positive beliefs and attitudes may have important indirect effects on cognitive enrichment because of their influence on the kinds of behaviors (for instance, exercise and mentally stimulating activities) that are known to be associated with cognitive enrichment.
More generally, individuals who are optimistic, agreeable, open to new experiences, conscientious, positively motivated and goal-directed are more likely to undergo successful aging, to take advantage of opportunities, to cope effectively with life circumstances, to effectively regulate emotional reactions to events, and to maintain a sense of well-being and life satisfaction in the face of challenge.
And just as maintaining some activity patterns in old age may reduce risk of cognitive decline, the persistence of other patterns of behavior may increase the risk. Chronic psychological distress—resulting from depression, anxiety, and negative emotions such as anger and shame—is associated with a variety of negative outcomes in adulthood, including cognitive decline. The tendency to experience psychological distress is often called neuroticism. Studies have consistently found a higher level of neuroticism to be linked to an increased incidence of Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment in old age.
Enriching cognition
Clearly, there is no magic pill that inoculates the individual against cognitive decline in old age. Thus, public policy should follow a health prevention model. Policy leaders might promote intellectual activities that are inherently meaningful for older adults, perhaps as embedded in larger social contexts (for example, the Elderhostel movement or adult continuing education). A critical issue for future research will be to understand how an engaged way of life can be promoted and implemented in midlife, during the working years. Given inevitable conflicts between work demands and time available for other roles (parenting, for one) and activities, it would be useful to know whether work-related activity programs (such as availability and use of physical exercise facilities at or near the workplace) could help foster an enriching lifestyle.
At the same time, the public must be aware that there is still much that is not known about cognitive fitness in old age, as well as some controversy about the magnitude and durability of mental exercise outcomes. People are marketing computer games and other means of exercising the mind, often making strong claims about the effectiveness of expensive products that have not been backed by actual scientific studies. Consumers should look for evidence demonstrating the benefits of such products—particularly with regard to everyday activities such as driving, memory for daily activities and better financial decision-making.
The next decades offer much promise for expanding our knowledge about aging and cognition. We may soon discover whether the limits on successful cognitive functioning in old age that were once seen as insurmountable can ultimately be viewed as pessimistic assumptions that focused on observable age-related decline rather than the potential for maximizing human performance through cognitive enrichment. Just as advances in medical science may lead to increased longevity through vehicles such as effective treatments for dementia-causing illnesses, advances in psychological science can make important contributions to improving the quality of life of long-living older adults. In part, these advances will empirically demonstrate that attitudes and behaviors can promote cognitive functioning in old age and, more generally, show how behavioral interventions can help us all age successfully.
The Power of Walking
Older adults who participated in aerobic exercise (walking) outperformed those in programs for stretching and toning (controls) in cognitive task areas: executive (related to planning and multitasking), controlled (effortful processes in response to novel situations), spatial (dealing with spatial information in perception or memory) and speed.
Source: “Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study,” by Stanley J. Colcombe and Arthur F. Kramer, in Psychological Science, Vol. 14, No. 2; March 2003
Capabilities over Time
An individual’s cognitive function can vary from maturity into old age. Although good habits can promote sound thinking within a given range, we cannot completely halt the effects of aging.
Top: Everyone experiences cognitive decline over a lifetime, but exercising the mind and body can help individuals stay above the middle (white dots) of the cognitive range (shaded area) and remain functional even beyond age 90.
Bottom: In one study of practice benefits, older adults (mean age 72, blue) were slower than younger adults (mean age 21, red) to search memory (comparison slope, vertical axis) to verify whether a word was one of a set they had committed to memory a short time earlier. But with enough practice, they could speedily identify target words without needing to search for them, producing zero slopes such as those produced by younger people.
Source for graph 1: “Enrichment Effects on Adult Cognitive Development: Can the Functional Capacity of Older Adults Be Preserved and Enhanced?” by C. Hertzog et al., in Psychological Science in the Public Interest, Vol. 9, No. 1; October 2008, © 2009 Association for Psychological Science; Source for graph 2: “Aging and Individual Differences in the Development of Skilled Memory Search Performance,” by C. Hertzog et al., in Psychology and Aging, Vol. 11, No. 3; 1996